Next Article in Journal
Integrating a New Dietetic Care Process in a Health Information System: A System and Process Analysis and Assessment
Previous Article in Journal
New Method of Avoiding Underestimation of Caries Incidence and Its Association with Possible Risk Factors in Japanese University Students: A Prospective Cohort Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Fair Play in a Context of Physical Education and Sports Behaviours

by
Mateusz Ludwiczak
* and
Małgorzata Bronikowska
Department of Recreation, Poznan University of Physical Education, 61-871 Poznan, Poland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(4), 2452; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19042452
Submission received: 17 January 2022 / Revised: 11 February 2022 / Accepted: 18 February 2022 / Published: 21 February 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Adolescents)

Abstract

:
The study examined prosocial behaviour and the perception of fair play in the context of physical education and sport (PES) in adolescents participating in sports, and those not undertaking sports at all. The aim of this study was to explore and indicate potential associations between an understanding of the concept of fair play and selected behaviours (e.g., abiding by the rules, obeying decisions of the referee and sportsmanship) in youth. In total, 1257 secondary school students, aged 16.1 ± 0.87 years (627 girls and 600 boys), were recruited. For assessing the level of fair play awareness (L), the Fair Play Moral Dimensions Concept Scale (FPMDCS) was used. To measure the declared level of prosocial behaviours of students, the survey My Physical Education Class (MPEC) was used. Participants were divided into groups in relation to training experience (no sports, amateur sports, professional sports) and subgroups, with levels according to their understanding of the Fair Play concept (L1 to L3). The results show significant difference in all dilemmas in various groups in relation to scores in subgroup L3 (p = 0.056; p = 0.012; p = 0.003) with regard to subgroups L2 and L1 in the level of understanding fair play. Overall, the percentage of respondents who view fair play only in the context of sport (L1) is 69%. Far fewer are those (31%) who understand it more broadly as a principle that applies to everyday life situations (L3). It seems to be crucial to promote moral development during PES activities, especially the value of respect.

1. Introduction

In recent years, it has increasingly been noted that school-aged students are less suited to situations that require social relationship competencies [1,2]. As a result, young people are spending less and less time with their peers, thus giving up the opportunity to build better social interactions [1]. It is therefore essential to create a favourable environment in PES contexts. It will allow youths to experience various situations and diverse dilemmas. However, it needs to be supported by prosocial reactions initiated by a PE teacher/sports coach, as well as being beneficial for promoting the ideals of sporting competition and moral behaviour. The Declaration on Sport, Tolerance and Fair Play agreed that sport is an important field of education [3]. Additionally, especially for young people, sport should serve primarily to convey universal values that help to raise the younger generation [2]. Sport, as a significant form of social activity, plays a key role on a global scale in the context of interpersonal communication and developing prosocial competencies [4,5,6,7]. As a universal phenomenon, sport affects all spheres of culture and, more importantly, educational systems and the values of young citizens and their behaviour [8]. Fair play as a common concept in sport ethics [9,10], one of the core principles of the Olympic Charter [11] and mission of the International Olympic Committee (I.O.C.), can also help in our understanding of justice in a broader social setting. Fair play should be considered not only in a sporting dimension. Additionally, as a part of everyday life, including all the desired behaviours of sportspersons in their social functioning [12]. On the other hand, unfortunately, sport in modern times is undergoing a crisis of values. With fouling now so fashionable, vicious and rule-breaking behaviour have, in recent decades, become not only visible and tolerated, but also something to be shown off [13]. Thus, it is important to prepare young people, skilfully and consciously, to serve their roles in adulthood fully and according to universal norms.
Age is one of the most critical variables to consider as moral judgment develops over time. A delay in the development of moral judgment leaves youths vulnerable, given increases in ego capabilities as well as peer stimuli from early through middle and late adolescence [14,15]. Moving from primary to secondary school leads the young individual into a new stage in the development of morality. Therefore, the individual begins to act according to the principles they recognize, regardless of the circumstances [16]. Satisfaction is gained by acting in accordance with standards, rather than by fear of punishment or the desire to receive a reward. Juveniles begin to consider the validity of actions taken and make their own moral judgments. Their moral judgments are based mostly on the intentions and motives of the offender.
A real dilemma, as a method of evaluating moral values based on hypothetical rather than theoretical dilemmas, can be of great environmental importance because participants can provide relevant situational content in relation to their everyday life and the cultural context that they experience [16]. To support the empirical validity of the theory, research has shown the hierarchical structure with objective measures of moral reasoning [17,18], which is crucial in accordance with cognitive development [19]. Reasoning is defined as a type of rationality directed towards deciding what to do and, when successful, issuing in an intention [20]. Hence, improvements in moral reasoning depend on instability in social interactions that result in a shift in perspective-taking.
Recent studies show that the influence of moral stages on the functioning of young people in life situations, such as mobbing at school, is negatively related to moral competencies [21]. There is evidence that adolescents are also characterized by a low level of moral competence, regardless of their participation in sports [22].
Moral principles are precursors to moral action, and the intention to perform an action, because they are motivational. More relevant models explaining moral behaviour include the four-component model of moral action [17], Bandura’s [23] social cognitive theory of moral thought and action, and the cognition model [24]. As Gibbons et al. [25] state, moral judgment and intentions are important indicators of one’s behaviour at a given moment. Moreover, Cheung et al. [18] showed that the higher the level of moral competence, the greater the positive influence on moral intentions. These findings confirm the view that moral awareness is motivational. Furthermore, awareness of personal interests is particularly responsible for immoral intentions. These results are consistent with the emphasis of cognitive-developmental theory on the cognitive basis of moral behaviour. The study clarifies the nature of moral behaviour as the embodiment of socially desirable principles—such as justice, social order and social progress—rather than as prosocial and altruistic behaviour centred around individual favour, e.g., being respectful to the teacher [26].
In a school setting, Olympic Education (OE) based on the neo-Olympism philosophy has been considered as a form of education [27,28,29,30]. An example of this might be also the Young Leaders Programme proposed by the IOC, which gives young people the opportunity to take responsibility for their environment. Through sport, they can develop the social dimension accordingly [31]. Whereas PES is a form of physical activity (PA) that allows children and adolescents to socialize and acquire social rules and values [32,33,34,35]. Studies on promoting Olympic values suggest an important and effective role of school in that process [27,28,29]. For instance, Parry [32] believes that the practice of sport, influenced by the philosophical anthropology of Olympism, offers a context and a route for PE teachers to achieve a number of important aims relating to moral education.
Ethics and education meet each other in the field of moral education. Most of the selected authors pointed out as valuable, albeit without carrying out further research and investigation on the matter [36,37,38]. Additionally, OE is a concept not only found within the Olympic Movement (IOC Agenda 2020 + 5 [39]. One recommendation to strengthen the role of sport is to promote Olympic values and social development programmes (including PE programmes for training teachers, sports coaches and volunteers). It is also becoming an integral part of the whole curriculum of elementary and secondary education in many countries [40,41,42]. The transfer of Olympic ethical values to education systems presents an opportunity to restore the position that PES deserves in general culture [13,28,43,44]. The need for OE results not only from the expectations of young people [32], but also from previous positive educational experiences in other countries like sport education [45,46]. For students who do not participate in sports, participation in PE classes is often the only way to learn fair play principals as a concept. Practicing sports, on the other hand, raises moral concerns for many of us.
Among the young, there is no difference in relation to gender in the understanding of the concept of fair play, in the context of participation in sport [42]. However, most young people perceive fair play only in the sphere of sport [47]. Values in sport may have important general implications for the intrinsic interest it provokes in participants. This analytic distinction covers most of the interpretations of the ideal and can serve as a normative starting point for further research.
Consequently, based on the abovementioned premises, we developed a study that aimed at exploring and indicating potential associations between levels of understanding of the concept of fair play and selected behavioural dilemmas (i.e., abiding by the rules, obeying decisions of the referee and sportsmanship) in young adolescents in the PES context. For that reason, we proposed two hypotheses: firstly, the teaching process of PES causes an increase in the level of positive prosocial behaviours and allows for a better understanding of the phenomenon of fair play among adolescences; secondly, despite different involvement in sports secondary school, students with the highest level of fair play indicate more positive prosocial behaviours in the context of the selected dilemmas.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Procedure

This study was conducted in the 2018/19 school year. It included 1257 secondary school students (no sports n = 197, amateur sports n = 496, professional sports n = 562) aged 16.1 ± 0.87 years (657 girls; 600 boys), recruited from 9 randomly selected schools in three major cities in the Wielkopolska Province in Poland. For the purpose of the study, we have proposed the following definitions: professional participation in sport meant engagement in regular system of competitions, organized by a sports federation [48], while in case of amateur sport, the most widespread definition is based on those individuals who practice sport casually as a hobby, for pure pleasure [49]. Permission for carrying out the research was secured separately and granted by the institutions concerned. The sample unit was a randomly selected school class. All participants taking part in an ‘in-person’ examination were informed about the study’s aims, risks and benefits, and parental consent was obtained before the study began. Questionnaires were completed in whole-class groups during a regular school lesson in quiet classroom conditions and took approximately 30 min to complete. All respondents were notified that their involvement would be anonymous and voluntary, and the fact that the results would only be used for scientific purposes. The investigator followed the study to assess the exposure and outcomes over a longer period of time [50]. This approach was aimed at defining the current relationships between the examined concepts in this study.
A cross-sectional study design was used. The research study was performed using the diagnostic survey method with the use of a structured survey questionnaire previously prepared, tested and assessed by competent evaluators. Those who agreed to participate in the project were requested to read and complete the inquiry form during regular school classes. Moreover, the respondents were informed of the option of withdrawing in case where they felt the need not to answer certain questions included in the questionnaire. The total return rate was 94%.

2.2. Fair Play Moral Dimensions Concept Scale (FPMDCS)

For assessing the level of fair play awareness (L), we used the three-level scale known as the Fair Play Moral Dimensions Concept Scale [42], designed based on Parry’s Fair Play Moral Dimensions Theory [11]. The scale contains three statements about the understanding of the concept of fair play, related to the levels (L1, L2, L3). The first level of fair play awareness (L1) is limited to the application of previously set/codified sporting rules during a sports competition (i.e., compliance with the rules that are the basic guideline for any sport and any kind of competition) [42]. The second level of fair play awareness (L2) refers not only to the application of codified sporting rules during a competition, but also to the employment of a code of behaviour resulting from the professional etiquette of those participating in competitive sport (i.e., efforts to compete in the right spirit, which means obeying written law that corresponds to generally accepted moral values) [42]. The third level in the application of the principle of fair play (L3) defines all the desired behaviours of the sportspersons by which they will stand out, not only in their sporting life, but also in their everyday life (i.e., fair play manifests itself in all situations as a life attitude, it is firmly anchored in the hierarchy of universal values) [42]. Responders were requested to mark one of the levels in accordance with their understanding of the principle of fair play, based on their own knowledge and previous experience. A conducted test–retest (following a two-week break) indicated a level of reliability of 0.78.

2.3. My Physical Education Class (MPEC)

To measure the declared prosocial behaviour of secondary school students, the My Physical Education Class (MPEC) questionnaire [27] was used, which demonstrated internal consistency (0.75) in an earlier study of a similar population [22]. The questionnaire is based on Horrocks’ [51] Pro-social Play Behaviour Inventory (HPPBI) and includes 10 moral dilemmas concerning sportsmanship behaviours, i.e., teasing others, showing off, arguing with teammates, complaining, sharing equipment, disobeying the rules of the game, ‘hogging’ the ball, disputing officials’ decisions, not taking turns and ignoring teammates’ suggestions for improving—all situated in the context of PES.
For this study, three dilemmas that are most closely related to the concept of fair play (teasing others, disobeying the rules of the game and disputing officials’ decisions) were selected, e.g., “When you win while playing in PE classes, you wonder whether to show off to your friends”. The questionnaire measures moral judgement, reasoning and the intentions of an individual through a self-rating system. Judgment and intentions are scored on a three-point Likert scale, and reasons on a five-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 = not at all to 5 = very much.

2.4. Ethics

The study was approved by the Bioethics Committee of the Karol Marcinkowski Medical University, Poznan (No. 893/18).

2.5. Statistic

Due to the lack of a normal distribution in the analysed groups, non-parametric statistical tools were employed. The chi-squared test was used to examine the independence of the qualitative factors from the type of group being examined, namely, those not practicing sport and those practicing sport at an amateur or professional level. The percentage distribution of indicators in the groups presented in tables also contains chi-squared statistical values and probability. If p < 0.05, the distribution of a given factor was dependent on the group, with it then becoming interesting in which categories a clear difference in the observed percentage would appear. A two-proportion z-test was used to test for a difference between two population proportions (differences between subgroups, i.e., L1–L2, L1–L3, L2–L3). All statistical analyses were completed using the STATISTICA ver. 13.1 software package (Stat Soft, Krakow, Poland).

3. Results

Table 1 presents the differences between levels of comprehension of the concept of fair play among students in relation to the dilemma, “When you win while playing in PE classes, you wonder whether to show off to your friends”, and at the same time the difference between subgroups, in terms of moral judgement, reasoning and intentions regarding their involvement in sports training (no sports training; amateur training; professional training).
Concerning the first question, in the ‘judgment’ category, data analysis indicates a tendency to significant difference in the group of professional sports-player responders (p = 0.056).
Participants from the L3 subgroup returned a higher result for the answer “it is not okay to show off in PE class” (58%), indicating the appropriate judgment in this specific moral context, in comparison to subgroups L2 (44%) (p = 0.011) and L1 (47%) (p = 0.056). Among the other groups—no sports and amateur sports—there were no significant differences among subgroups.
Analysis of the second question, relating to ‘reasoning’ as a component of the first dilemma, did not show any statistically significant differences in any group. The most frequent declaration among all surveyed groups at all levels of understanding of the concept of fair play was the response: “it is nice or not nice to show off”.
The analysis of the study results for the third question, in the ‘intentions’ category of the first dilemma, did not indicate a statistical difference in the declarations of any of the study groups. Levels of fair play did not affect the differences between participants in relation to prosocial behaviours. All groups of students mostly declare that they will “never show off” in future PE class, which is believed to be the model attitude. In this dilemma, the vast majority of professional athletes responded to the moral judgement aspect with appropriate sportsmanship behaviours.
Table 2 presents the difference between the levels of comprehension of the concept of fair play among students in relation to the second dilemma, which concerns the declared attitude of the respondents in the context of following the rules of the game when playing in PE class. Likewise, in Table 1, the differences between subgroups were also presented to see if there are any significant differences.
Analysis of the research results concerning the first question, in the ‘judgment’ category, indicates statistical significance among amateur sports-players (p = 0.012). Most participants in all subgroups declared “it is not okay not to follow the games rules during PE lessons” as an answer to this dilemma, though students from subgroup L1 (60%) and L2 (61%) returned a statistically significant lower percentage of such responses than did subgroup L3 (74%) (L3–L1; p = 0.011; L3–L2; p = 0.011). Subgroup L3 also returned a lower percentage (8%) of the reverse answer “it is okay not to follow the games rules during PE lessons” among amateur sports-players in comparison with subgroups L2 (11%) and L1 (18%) (L3–L1; p = 0.001; L3–L2; p = 0.001).
In the ‘reasoning’ category of the second dilemma, there were no statistically significant differences in any of the levels of understanding of the principle of fair play. In all groups, participants tended to the response “it is fair or not fair to follow the rules”.
Analysis of the results for the third question, in the ‘intentions’ category of this second dilemma, indicates marginal significance in the no-sports group (p = 0.059). L3 participants declared that in future, they will “never disobey rules” (61%). This was a significantly higher result than among students from L2 (53%) and L1 (60%). In the L3 subgroup, only 3% declared that they will “disobey rules most of the time”. Groups of amateur and professional sports-players also declared prosocial attitudes in relation to disobeying rules. In relation to dilemma, amateur athletes maintained similar reactions across all levels of understanding of fair play. In intentions of students not practicing sport could noticed a positive outcome in relation to moral behaviours.
Table 3 presents the difference between levels of comprehension of fair play among students in relation to the dilemma concerning the student’s declarative attitude towards a decision made by the referee while playing in PE (i.e., to question the decision or not) as well the difference between subgroups to show significant differences.
Analysis of the research results concerning the first question, in the ‘judgment’ category, indicates a statistical difference in the group of professional sports-players (p = 0.003). In this group, students from the subgroup with the strongest understanding of the principle of fair play (L3) declared that it is “not OK to dispute a referee’s decision in PE class” at a higher percentage (28%)—statistically significant in relation to subgroup L1 (17%) and L2 (20%). In subgroup L1, the percentage answering that “it is OK to dispute a referee’s decision in PE class” (27%) was even higher than the percentage answering that “it is not OK to dispute a referee’s decision in PE class” (17%).
In the analysis of the second question of the third dilemma, concerning ‘reasoning’, a statistical difference was noticed among amateur sports-players (p = 0.003). In subgroup L1, the most common answer was “get punished” (43%); in L2 it was “against the rules” (31%); students from L3 returned a higher result (32%) for the answer “is it fair or not to dispute a referee’s decision in PE class”, indicating hesitation that might be caused by specific environmental conditions (e.g., upbringing, experience, knowledge, etc.).
As shown by the above analysis, results concerning the ‘intentions’ category for the third dilemma did not indicate a statistical difference between any of the levels of understanding of fair play principles. There is a clear tendency, however, to intend to make decisions about disputing with the referee “sometimes”, leaving the decision to circumstances (48–62% through all stages of fair play understanding in all study-groups). On the other hand, the low percentage (24–37%) of those declaring compliance with the referee’s decision during the game in any situation seems disturbing. Assessing the dilemma concerning the acceptance of referee decisions, professional sports-players indicated an attitude undermining the role of a referee.

4. Discussion

Sport has influence on social skills and, used properly, could teach competition, cooperation, role-playing and discipline regarding rules, regulations and goals [52]. It can be also seen as a laboratory of human experience [38]. Sport is supposed to create a system of value based on the experiences of participants via training. PE as a school subject consists of the core principles of sport and provides an opportunity to develop students, not only physically, but also morally and socially. Therefore, a part of the didactic process in many countries is OE, with particular emphasis on the role of fair play as the overriding and universal principle [13,30,32,41,43,44]. It is assumed that fair play as a part of OE should develop moral competencies in terms of judgment, reasoning and intentions [44]. According to our hypothesis the results of this study show that the level of understanding fair play has a crucial role in behaviour related to moral dilemmas. In each situation participants from subgroup L3, with the highest understanding of the concept of fair play, showed a higher level of positive prosocial behaviour in relation to judgment, reasoning and intensions in comparison to students from subgroups L1 and L2 (representing the lowest and moderate levels of understanding of fair play). The fair play attitude and awareness influences prosocial behaviour and relations in teams on and off the field [53], and also reduces antisocial behaviour among adolescents [54]. Similar results were presented by Vidoni and Ward [55], who claim that socially competent players support each other’s performance, show respect to opponents and officials and, furthermore, accept losses without complaint and win without gloating. On the contrary, antisocial behaviours (e.g., cheating, gamesmanship, doping) violating the rules or the spirit of the game have become a part of the game [56], with those athletes using various forms of moral disengagement to differentiate between deception and ‘clever’ play, or which can lead to other unwanted antisocial behaviours in school, such as bullying or cheating [42]. In our study, in the first and second dilemmas, we noticed well-represented prosocial behaviours among most participants in all variables. By contrast, in the third dilemma relating to referees’ decisions, we observed that a majority of participants were uncertain in their declaration, contrasting with Vidoni and Ward’s study [55] in which athletes showed respect to officials. We must, therefore, note that sports participants might be influenced in their moral development in both a positive and a negative way [57]. For this reason, PE teachers and sport coaches, through their teaching/coaching style, play a crucial role in the upbringing and sound education of the younger generation [58]. Young people who are socially competent react properly to situations occurring in everyday life by managing conflicts and dealing appropriately with any disagreements [59]. With this in mind, it seems important to know how students perceive fair play and if it influences their experiences, in a way related to PE class. In the case of PE, the problem may be rooted in a still-present misconception in understanding the modern paradigm of PE, namely, in that a traditional physical sport-centred approach is still more present than a holistic approach, which would focus on enhancing social attitudes [60], moral development, and life-long learning skills [22]. Kowalska [61] noticed that changes in behaviour in relation to sports situations also change behaviour in everyday life dilemmas. At the same time, she claims that, for a minor group of students (26.2%), fair play is more than just a sports principle [47]. In her study, half of the responders believed that fair play is respected by people, regardless of whether they participate in sports or not. Conversely, 31% of young respondents did not see any relation between fair play and everyday life situations. Comparing the above-mentioned conclusions to our research, we can highlight the number of participants who perceive fair play either only in the context of sport settings (69%) or more broadly as a rule concerning situations in everyday life (31%).
Among young people in sport, the most frequent display of sportsmanship [62] is the players’ greeting after the game. This confirms the right attitude towards other players and recognizes showing off as an undesirable behaviour. Peer violence has a major impact on the self-esteem of young people [63], and practicing sports in an appropriate manner could reduce this problem [63]. Some studies indicate that young athletes, in comparison to non-sporting school students, have lower levels of aggression [63], therefore behaving properly both within and beyond sporting situations. This outcome corresponds with our research. The results show that students participating in professional sports training understand fair play, not only in sport, but also beyond sports situations. They are in line with other authors [42,47].
Previous research [64,65] has shown that violence against referees has become more common, which can lead to dismissive behaviour in relation to umpires. Our results show that most young people consider undermining a referee’s decision even during PE classes. Krebs and Denton [66] argue that people invoke moral judgment afterwards to justify what they have done. Furthermore, these authors claim that moral judgment stages are only weakly associated with behaviour and may be used in a flexible way in accordance with different social contexts or reasons. They argue [66] that optimal moral judgment does not reflect adequacy but usefulness, that is, ‘creating the conditions that enable people to achieve their goals and advance their interests in cooperative ways’ [66] (p. 646). In fact, different kinds of contexts pull toward different forms of moral judgment. In this sense, people move in and out of moral orders, not stages of moral development. Other authors [67,68] show interesting examples of flexible stage use, providing evidence that higher moral judgment competence increases resistance to low-stage pulling contexts or sets the stage for moral action. In our study, the difference among students is related to reasoning about dilemmas and depends on the level of understanding of the concept of fair play. In terms of moral reasoning related to the decisions of referees, the results raise our curiosity. Among amateur sports-players, each fair play comprehension level showed a different reasoning pattern. This might be associated with the developing brains of adolescents, information processing capacity and working memory can be disturbed by a growing reward and control system, leading to risk-taking or unethical behaviours [69]. These limitations have not always been challenged effectively, particularly in terms of PES. Shields and Bredemeier [28], in their research on school sport, claim that PES, if aimed at ‘winning at all cost’, may cause problems such as cheating, arguing with officials and egotistical celebration. In relation to ‘game reasoning’ [28], players react with a lower moral reasoning stage during participation in sport, but, when they understand that issues are related to everyday life, they shift to more mature reasoning. In our results for the dilemma relating to referee decisions, in the reasoning aspect, we can see some correlation between answers at each level of fair play comprehension and the stages of Kohlberg’s scale [13]. For students from subgroup L1 the most frequent answer was “get punished”, whereas, for subgroup L2, it was “against the rules” and, amongst participants representing the highest comprehension group, subgroup L3, “fair or not fair” was the most declared statement. These results align with previous research by Shields and Bredemeier [28] and Bronikowska et al. [22].
The explanation of our results might be that the process of learning values in that group of school students was not conducted appropriately and most probably lacked guidance by established theories, such as those of Koh [70] or Kohlberg [14], or may not have been trained at all. In addition, differences between sports education in PE contexts and in extra sports activities, written about successively by Vella et al. [71] and Lee [72], may have affected our results. Situated within this context, our results may come as no surprise, but they do call for more training on moral issues for students during school years, especially since previous research [64,65] showed that this is not a problem only in youth sport but across all age categories in sporting competitions. This aspect is worth deeper consideration in future.
Some limitations of this study need to be acknowledged. There are cognition measures (multiple-choice questionnaires to evaluate moral judgment representing different stages) using hypothetical dilemmas (moral competence, not moral performance). These factors might affect the findings. The study was based on a declarative assessment of the participants’ attitudes, whereas real-life situations might be a valuable check for participants’ real values. Hence, our next step in further research will be to design an adequate intervention programme, considering the dilemmas and factors affecting evaluation in the PES environment.

5. Conclusions

This study showed that students recognized dilemmas mostly with prosocial behaviour and that this was dependent on the presented level of fair play amongst responders in relation to moral judgement, reasoning and intentions. In the dilemma concerning relations with classmates, the vast majority of professional athletes responded to the moral judgement aspect with appropriate sportsmanship behaviours. Moreover, in the dilemma concerning abiding by the rules, amateur athletes maintained similar reactions across all levels of understanding of fair play. In the same dilemma, the intentions of students not practicing sport showed a positive outcome in relation to moral behaviours.
In the last dilemma, concerning the acceptance of referee decisions, professional sports-players indicated an attitude undermining the role of a referee. Previous research [65,66] showed that this is not a problem only in youth sport but across all age categories in sporting competitions. This aspect is worth deeper consideration in future. In terms of moral reasoning related to the decisions of referees, the results raise our wonder. Among those who declared themselves as amateur sports-players, each fair play comprehension level showed a different reasoning pattern.
It seems to be crucial to promote moral development during PES activities, especially respect for other people—teammates, and the referee. It is worth emphasizing that an understanding of fair play determined/mitigated outcomes in all dilemmas in each group of students. Therefore, in the process of education, fair play should be properly related to the principles of functioning in everyday life. Young people need a sense of understanding and a suitable level of awareness in order to get involved in undertaking a life-long PA.
It is also worth considering the creation of a pedagogical intervention which could help students to develop their moral competencies and prosocial behaviours, in relation to recent research and the low level of moral competence among the young. As a final remark, we should say that educating one to practice a behaviour of fair play as one of the most universal principles in sport and that goes beyond sport is, or at least should be, one of the most fundamental duties of modern education, not just through school PE classes, but throughout the schooling system. This calls also for adjustments in curricula aimed at suitably preparing future teachers for the proper implementation of this ideal in the school environment, where the sensitization of young people to the concept of a ‘clean game’ during school studies will hopefully shape their moral outlooks and have an impact on their adult lives in a fundamental manner.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.B. and M.L.; methodology, M.B. and M.L.; software, M.B. and M.L.; validation, M.L. and M.B.; formal analysis, M.L. and M.B.; investigation, M.L. and M.B.; resources, M.L. and M.B.; data curation, M.B. and M.L.; writing—original draft preparation, M.L.; writing—review and editing, M.B.; visualization, M.L. and M.B.; supervision, M.B.; project administration, M.L. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Bioethics Committee of the Karol Marcinkowski Medical University, Poznan (no. 893/18).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author, upon reasonable request.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Lancy, D.F.; Grove, M.A. Marbles and Machiavelli: The role of game play in children’s social development. Am. J. Play 2017, 3, 489–499. [Google Scholar]
  2. Buchs, C.; Filippou, D.; Pulfrey, C.; Volpé, Y. Challenges for cooperative learning implementation: Reports from elementary school teachers. J. Educ. Teach. 2017, 43, 296–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Council of Europe. Declaration on Sport, Tolerance and Fair Play. Available online: http://www.coe.int/t/dg4/sport/resources/texts/spdecl_en.asp (accessed on 21 November 2021).
  4. Izquierdo, C.; Anguera, M.T. The Analysis of Interpersonal Communication in Sport From Mixed Methods Strategy: The Integration of Qualitative-Quantitative Elements Using Systematic Observation. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 637304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Cronin, L.D.; Allen, J. Examining the relationships among the coaching climate, life skills development and well-being in sport. Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 2018, 13, 815–827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Graupensperger, S.A.; Jensen, C.J.; Evans, M.B. A meta-analytic review of studies using the Prosocial and Antisocial Behavior in Sport Scale: Associations among intergroup moral behaviors. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol. 2018, 7, 186–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Serrano-Durá, J.; Molina, P.; Martínez-Baena, A. Systematic review of research on fair play and sporting competition. Sport Educ. Soc. 2021, 26, 648–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Simon, R.L.; Torres, C.R.; Hager, P.H. Fair Play. The Ethics of Sports, 4th ed.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  9. Boixados, M.A.; Cruz, J.; Torregrossa, M.; Valiente, L. Relationships among Motivational Climate, Satisfaction, Perceived Ability, and Fair Play Attitudes in Young Soccer Players. J. Appl. Sport Psychol. 2004, 16, 301–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lipoński, W. Landmarks in British History and Culture. A Monograph of Selected Issues, 1st ed.; Wydawnictwo Naukowe UAM: Poznań, Poland, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  11. Olympic Charter. Available online: https://stillmed.olympic.org/media/Document%20Library/OlympicOrg/General/EN-Olympic-Charter.pdf (accessed on 12 October 2020).
  12. Parry, J. The Moral and Cultural Dimensions of Olympism and their Educational Application. In 100 Years as from the Foundation of the IOC: Commitments of the International Olympic Movement Towards the Modern Society of the 21st Century; Georgiadis, K., Ed.; International Olympic Academy: Olympia, Greece, 1995; pp. 181–195. [Google Scholar]
  13. Culpan, I. Olympism, physical education, and critical pedagogy. Eur. Phys. Educ. Rev. 2019, 5, 847–858. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Kohlberg, L. Moral stages and moralization: The cognitive-development approach. In Moral Development and Behavior: Theory Research and Social Issues, 1st ed.; Lickona, T., Ed.; Holt, Rinehart and Winston: New York, NY, USA, 1976; pp. 31–53. [Google Scholar]
  15. Gavaghan, M.P.; Arnold, K.D.; Gibbs, J.C. Moral judgment in delinquents and nondelinquents: Recognition versus production measures. J. Psychol. 1983, 114, 267–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Gibbs, J.C.; Basinger, K.S.; Fuller, D. Moral Maturity: Measuring the Development of Sociomoral Reflection; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  17. Rest, J. Moral Development: Advances in Research and Theory; Paeger: New York, NY, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
  18. Cheung, C.K.; Chan, W.T.; Lee, T.Y.; Liu, S.C.; Leung, K.K. Structure of moral consciousness and moral intentions among youth in Hong Kong. Int. J. Adolesc. Youth 2001, 9, 83–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Kohlberg, L. The Philosophy of Moral Development Moral Stages and the Idea of Justice, 1st ed.; Harper & Row: San Francisco, CA, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  20. Stanford Encyclopaedia of Philosophy. Moral Reasoning. Available online: https://plato.stanford.edu/entries/reasoning-moral/#DefiMoraReas (accessed on 15 September 2021).
  21. Doehne, M.; von Grundherr, M.; Schäfer, M. Peer influence in bullying: The autonomy-enhancing effect of moral competence. Aggress. Behav. 2018, 44, 591–600. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Bronikowska, M.; Korcz, A.; Krzysztoszek, J.; Bronikowski, M. How Years of Sport Training Influence the Level of Moral Competences of Physical Education and Sport Students. Biomed. Res. Int. 2019, 4, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Bandura, A. Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process 1991, 50, 248–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Candee, D.; Kohlberg, L. Moral judgment and moral action: A reanalysis of Haan, Smith, and Block’s (1968) Free Speech Movement data. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1987, 52, 554–564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Gibbons, S.L.; Ebbeck, V. The effect of different teaching strategies on the moral development of physical education students. J. Teach. Phys. Educ. 1997, 17, 85–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Bond, M.H.; Chi, V.M.Y. Values and moral behavior in mainland China. Psychol. Int. J. Psychol. Orient 1997, 40, 251–264. [Google Scholar]
  27. Gibbons, S.; Ebbeck, V.; Weiss, M. Fair Play for Kids: Effects on the Moral Development of Children in Physical Education. Res. Q. Exerc. Sport. 1995, 66, 247–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Shields, D.L.; Bredemeier, B.J. Character Development and Physical Activity, 1st ed.; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
  29. Grammatikopoulos, V.; Hassandra, M.; Koustelios, A.; Theodoriakis, Y. Evaluating the Olympic education program: A qualitative approach. Stud. Educ. Eval. 2005, 31, 347–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Podstawa Programowa. Podstawa Programowa Kształcenia Ogólnego dla Czteroletniego Liceum Ogólnokształcącego i Pięcioletniego Technikum z Przedmiotu Wychowanie Fizyczne. Available online: https://podstawaprogramowa.pl/Liceum-technikum/Wychowanie-fizyczne (accessed on 30 July 2021).
  31. IOC Young Leaders. Be the Change through Sport. Available online: https://stillmed.olympics.com/media/Document%20Library/OlympicOrg/IOC/What-We-Do/Promote-Olympism/Young-Leaders/Young-Leaders.pdf (accessed on 14 January 2022).
  32. Parry, J. Physical Education as Olympic Education. Eur. Phy. Educ. 1998, 4, 56–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  33. Binder, D. Teaching Olympism in Schools: Olympic Education as a Focus on Values Education. Bello Tera: Centre d’Estudis Olimpics (UAB). International Chair in Olympism (IOC-UAB). 2005. Available online: http://olympicstudies.uab.es/lec/pdf/binder.pdf (accessed on 14 May 2021).
  34. Naul, R. Olympic Education, 2nd ed.; Meyer & Meyer Sport: Oxford, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  35. Weiss, M.R.; Smith, A.L.; Stuntz, C.P. Moral Development in Sport and Physical Activity: Theory, Research, and Intervention, 3rd ed.; T.S. Horn: Champaign, IL, USA, 2008; pp. 187–210. [Google Scholar]
  36. Power, F.C.; Higgins-D’Alessandro, A. The just community approach to moral education and the moral atmosphere of the school. In Moral and Character Education, 1st ed.; Nucci, L., Narvaez, D., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 230–247. [Google Scholar]
  37. Opstoel, K.; Chapelle, L.; Prins, F.J.; De Meester, A.; Haerens, L.; van Tartwijk, J.; De Martelaer, K. Personal and social development in physical education and sports: A review study. Eur. Phy. Educ. 2020, 26, 797–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Pennington, C. Moral development and sportsmanship in interscholastic sports and physical education. J. Phys. Educ. Recreat. Danc. 2017, 88, 36–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Olympic Agenda 2020+5, 15 Recommendations. Available online: https://stillmed.olympics.com/media/Document%20Library/OlympicOrg/IOC/What-We-Do/Olympic-agenda/Olympic-Agenda-2020-5-15-recommendations.pdf (accessed on 14 January 2022).
  40. Culpan, I.; Wigmore, S. The Delivery of Olympism Education within a Physical Education Context Drawing on Critical Pedagogy. Int. J. Sport Health Sci. 2010, 8, 67–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Naul, R.; Binder, D.; Rychtecký, A.; Culpan, I. Olympic education as an academic study. In Olympic Education: An international review, 1st ed.; Naul, R., Binder, D., Rychtecký, A., Culpan, I., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 344–350. [Google Scholar]
  42. Bronikowska, M.; Korcz, A.; Pluta, B.; Krzysztoszek, J.; Ludwiczak, M.; Łopatka, M.; Wawrzyniak, S.; Kowalska, J.E.; Bronikowski, M. Fair play in physical education and beyond. Sustainability 2019, 11, 7064. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Bronikowski, M. Etyczne aspekty wychowawczego programu edukacji olimpijskiej. Wnioski z badań. Rocz. Nauk. AWF Gdan. 2001, 10, 141–173. [Google Scholar]
  44. Glapa, A.; Bronikowski, M.; Laudańska-Krzemińska, I. Do students really need Olympic education at school? Stud. Sport Hum. 2016, 19, 30–36. [Google Scholar]
  45. Siedentop, D. Sport Education: Quality Physical Education through Positive Sport Experiences, 1st ed.; Human Kinetics: Champaign, IL, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
  46. Navarro Domínguez, B.; Cerrada Nogales, J.A.; Abad Robles, M.T.; Giménez Fuentes-Guerra, F.J. The Development of Fair Play in Physical Education and School Sports: A Systematic Review. Eur. J. Contemp. Educ. 2021, 10, 308–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Kowalska, J.E. Fair play i faul play w swiadomosci uczniow Greig City Academy w Londynie. Awareness of fair play and foul play among the students of Greig City Academy in London. J. Educ. Health Sport 2015, 5, 457–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Bronikowska, M.; Korcz, A.; Bronikowski, M. The role of sports practice in young adolescent development of moral competence. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 5324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Collins English Dictionary. Definition if “Amateurism”. Available online: https://www.collinsdictionary.com/dictionary/english/amateurism (accessed on 12 January 2022).
  50. Setia, M.S. Methodology series module 3: Cross-sectional studies. Indian J. Dermatol. 2016, 61, 261–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Horrocks, R.N. Sportsmanship: Moral reasoning. Phys. Educ. 1980, 37, 208–212. [Google Scholar]
  52. Bloom, G.A.; Smith, M.D. Hockey violence: A test of cultural spillover theory. Sociol. Sport J. 1996, 13, 65–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Rutten, E.A.; Schuengel, C.; Dirks, E.; Stams, G.J.J.; Biesta, G.J.; Hoeksma, J.B. Predictors of antisocial and prosocial behavior in an adolescent sports context. Soc. Dev. 2011, 20, 294–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Rutten, E.A.; Deković, M.; Stams, G.J.J.; Schuengel, C.; Hoeksma, J.B.; Biesta, G.J. On-and off-field antisocial and prosocial behavior in adolescent soccer players: A multilevel study. J. Adolesc. 2008, 31, 371–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Vidoni, C.; Ward, P. Effects of a dependent group-oriented contingency on middle school physical education students’ fair play behaviors. J. Behav. Educ. 2006, 15, 80–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Palou, P.; Ponseti, F.X.; Cruz, J.; Vidal, J.; Cantallops, J.; Borras, P.A.; Garcia-Mas, A. Acceptance of gamesmanship and cheating in young competitive athletes in relation to the motivational climate generated by parents and coaches. Percept. Mot. Skill. 2013, 117, 290–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Cruz, J.; Ponseti, F.X.; Sampaio, M.; Gamito, J.M.; Marques, A.; Vinas, H.; Borrueco, M.; Carvalho, L.; Garcia-Mas, A. Effects of a Psychology–Based training programme on football grassroots coaches upon young player’s sportspersonship and disposition to cheat. J. Sport Psychol. 2018, 27, 23–27. [Google Scholar]
  58. Hodge, K.; Lonsdale, C. Prosocial and antisocial behavior in sport: The role of coaching style, autonomous vs controlled motivation, and moral disengagement. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2011, 33, 527–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Sheridan, S. Building social skills in the classroom. In Understanding and Managing Children’s Classroom Behavior, 1st ed.; Goldstein, S., Ed.; Wiley Interscience Press: Indianapolis, IN, USA, 1995; pp. 375–396. [Google Scholar]
  60. Koszałka-Silska, A.; Korcz, A.; Wiza, A. The Impact of Physical Education Based on the Adventure Education Programme on Self-esteem and Social Competences of Adolescent Boys. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 3021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Kowalska, J.E. The principle of fair play in the aspect of responsibility in the opinion of junior high schools students-Supporting sport clubs in Lodz. J. Educ. Health Sport 2018, 8, 836–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Saenz Ibanez, A.; Gimeno Marco, F.; Gutierrez Pablo, H.; Lacambra Correas, D. Evaluation of the Sportsmanship-Aggression Continuum in Youth Football. Rev. Psicol. Deporte 2019, 28, 33–40. [Google Scholar]
  63. Milovanović, I.; Roklicer, R.; Drid, P. The relation between youth sport and the reduction of peer violence. FU. Phys. Ed. Sport. 2019, 17, 479–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Bowker, A.; Boekhoven, B.; Nolan, A.; Bauhaus, S.; Glover, P.; Powell, T.; Taylor, S. Naturalistic observations of spectator behavior at youth hockey games. Sport Psychol. 2009, 23, 301–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Dolan, P.; Connolly, J. Emotions, Violence and Social Belonging: An Elysian Analysis of Sports Spectatorship. Br. J. Sociol. 2014, 48, 284–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Krebs, D.L.; Denton, K. Toward a More Pragmatic Approach to Morality: A Critical Evaluation of Kohlberg’s Model. Psychol. Rev. 2005, 112, 629–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  67. Gibbs, J.C. Moral Development and Reality: Beyond the Theories of Kohlberg, Hoffman, and Haidt, 4th ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  68. Pizarro, D.A.; Bloom, P. The intelligence of the moral intuitions: A comment on Haidt. Psychol. Rev. 2003, 110, 193–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Steinberg, L. Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 16, 55–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Koh, K.T.; Ong, S.W.; Camiré, M. Implementation of a values training program in physical education and sport: Perspectives from teachers, coaches, students, and athletes. Phys. Educ. Sport. Pedagog. 2016, 21, 295–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Vella, S.A.; Oades, L.G.; Crowe, T.P. A pilot test of transformational leadership training for sports coaches: Impact on the developmental experiences of adolescent athletes. Int. J. Sports Sci. Coach. 2013, 8, 513–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Lee, M. Values and responsibilities in childrens sports. Phys. Act. Rev. 1988, 11, 19–27. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Student attitudes to peer relations during PE classes, considering their level of involvement in sporting activities and their level of comprehension of the concept of fair play (%).
Table 1. Student attitudes to peer relations during PE classes, considering their level of involvement in sporting activities and their level of comprehension of the concept of fair play (%).
Dilemma 1When You Win while Playing in PE Classes, You Wonder whether to Show Off to Your Friends.
No Sports n = 199Amateur Sports n = 496Professional Sports n = 562
Level of FP (L)L1
n = 52
L2
n = 85
L3
n = 62
L1
n = 127
L2
n = 205
L3
n = 164
L1
n = 140
L2
n = 262
L3
n = 160
JudgmentDo you think that it is OK to show off in PE class?
Ok1081317139121413
Sometimes384831313439414229 *,B,C
Not Ok524456525352474458 *,B,C
Ch2 (p value)4.87 (0.300)5.27 (0.259)9.17 (0.056)
ReasoningWhich is the most important thing to consider when you decide whether it is OK to show off?
Get punished8551054757
Get even968131010101610
Nice or not nice444842435244534249
Against rules875665664
Fair or not313440282737243130
Chi2 (p value)2.73 (0.948)11.71 (0.164)9.5 (0.300)
IntentionsIf you win games in future PE class, what do you think you will do?
Never show off655868666163575462
Sometimes293724243331363828
Show off most of the times65810667810
Chi2 (p value)3.53 (0.472)4.42 (0.350)5.62 (0.223)
* Notes significant difference between subgroups, p < 0.05, L1–L3 B, L2–L3 C.
Table 2. Student attitudes during PE classes in relation to obeying the rules of the game, with respect to their level of involvement in sporting activities and their level of comprehension of the concept of fair play (%).
Table 2. Student attitudes during PE classes in relation to obeying the rules of the game, with respect to their level of involvement in sporting activities and their level of comprehension of the concept of fair play (%).
Dilemma 2When You Participate in Different Games during Your PE Classes, You Wonder whether to Follow Their Rules.
No Sports n = 199Amateur Sports n = 496Professional Sports n = 562
Level of FP (L)L1
n = 52
L2
n = 85
L3
n = 62
L1
n = 127
L2
n = 205
L3
n = 164
L1
n = 140
L2
n = 262
L3
n = 160
JudgementDo you think it is okay not to follow the games rules during PE lessons?
Ok2117818118 *,B,C151515
Sometimes252215222818 *,C262426
Not Ok546177606174 *,B,C606159
Chi2 (p value)7.80 (0.098)12.77 (0.012) *0.27 (0.991)
ReasoningWhich is the most important thing to consider when you decide whether it is OK to follow the rules?
Get punished251918201411341815
Get even4176441178
Nice or not nice141715171620191315
Against rules212819272321211822
Fair or not373542304244294440
Chi2 (p value)5.37 (0.716)11.33 (0.181)12.56 (0.127)
IntentionsIf you are playing games in future PE classes, what do you think you will do?
Never disobey rules605361545868506057
Sometimes353836373525423438
Disobey rules most of the time693 *,B,C977865
Chi2 (p value)2.75 (0.059)7.20 (0.125)4.49 (0.343)
* Notes significant difference between subgroups, p < 0.05, L1–L3 B, L2–L3 C.
Table 3. Student attitudes to the referee’s decision made during the PE lesson, considering their level of involvement in sporting activities and their level of comprehension of the concept of fair play (%).
Table 3. Student attitudes to the referee’s decision made during the PE lesson, considering their level of involvement in sporting activities and their level of comprehension of the concept of fair play (%).
Dilemma 3When a Referee Decides during a Game in Your PE Class, You Wonder whether to Dispute the Decision.
No Sports n = 199Amateur Sports n = 496Professional Sports n = 562
Level of FP (L)L1
n = 52
L2
n = 85
L3
n = 62
L1
n = 127
L2
n = 205
L3
n = 164
L1
n = 140
L2
n = 262
L3
n = 160
JudgementDo you think that it is OK to dispute a referee’s decision in PE class?
Ok191715241918271623
Sometimes696660546056566549
Not Ok121826222226172028 *,B,C
Chi2 (p value)3.97 (0.409)2.81 (0.589)15.68 (0.003) *
ReasoningWhich is the most important thing to consider when you decide whether to dispute a referee’s decision?
Get punished3724274328 *,A24 *,B413133
Get even1080275864
Nice or not nice141521161115141316
Against rules2326211931 *,A24292923
Fair or not172731202432 *,B212123
Chi2 (p value)9.23 (p = 0.322)22.11 (0.003) *11.76 (0.162)
IntentionsIf a referee makes decision in future PE classes, what do you think you will do?
Never dispute231724231419202317
Sometimes504848536255495750
Dispute most of the times273527242426313733
Chi2 (p value)2.31 (0.678)5.67 (0.224)0.60 (0.962)
* Notes significant difference between subgroups, p < 0.05, L1–L2 A, L1–L3 B, L2–L3 C.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ludwiczak, M.; Bronikowska, M. Fair Play in a Context of Physical Education and Sports Behaviours. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 2452. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19042452

AMA Style

Ludwiczak M, Bronikowska M. Fair Play in a Context of Physical Education and Sports Behaviours. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(4):2452. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19042452

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ludwiczak, Mateusz, and Małgorzata Bronikowska. 2022. "Fair Play in a Context of Physical Education and Sports Behaviours" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 4: 2452. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19042452

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop